Thursday March 6, 2003
Session 4: Beyond Therapy: Better Memories?
Discussion of two papers:
(1) "Why Remember?" by Gilbert Meilaender, and
(2) Staff Working Paper, "'Better' Memories?
The Promise and Peils of Pharmacological Interventions"
CHAIRMAN KASS: The last session today is revisiting
on the topic of better memories, the promise and the peril of pharmacological
intervention.
And we have here a staff background paper which, before taking
up the technologies now and perhaps soon to be available to intervene
in human memories, talks about what memory is in its heterogeneity,
talks a lot about what a better memory might be and how difficult
it is to specify that; different ways in which memory fails or fails
to please us, grouping them amongst lost memories which might be
restored, weak memories which might be enhanced, and then bad memories
which one might choose to transform.
And that paper concludes by suggesting that perhaps it is this
last category, the blunting of the affective aspects of memories
of painful, unpleasant memories that might be the technology most
upon us.
And, in fact, in the session that we had with Dr. McGaugh and
Dr. Schachter, there was some discussion about that prospect in
the follow-up discussion.
And then we have also as background, as I think the point of departure
from this discussion, a paper that Gil Meilaender wrote really in
response to a request and a conversation that we had after the last
meeting, which takes up this last possibility and raises the question
as to why should we remember especially those things for which a
case might be made that we'd be better off not remembering it.
I don't want to have a discussion aimed primarily about any
of the papers precisely, and Gil has asked that this not be a sort
of back and forth interrogation of what he said, but the paper really
is to be used to prompt a discussion aimed at an increased clarification
and understanding of the particular issue.
And, once again, I'm going to try and this time I hope to
produce a question that's less easily discussed and more readily
engaged.
Let's put it this way. We have, although the obliteration
of all pain would produce a kind of bodily disaster, as Professor
Pinker already mentioned, but we do have analgesia and for certain
kinds of severe infliction of pain, we even have anesthesia, and
the question is why shouldn't one think about analgesia for
the memory.
Why should we have to live with painful, guilty, shame- filled,
shocking or unpleasant memories if we don't have to and science
could offer us a way out of it?
That's a question that Gil offers a kind of argument for,
an argument which he can make for himself or we can draw on if we
wish, but I think that's the question that the paper is trying
to get us to think about, and I would simply like to pose the question
flatly and see where we go.
Mike.
DR. GAZZANIGA: Have you got any Ritalin?
(Laughter.)
CHAIRMAN KASS: Look. If this --
DR. GAZZANIGA: A fascinating thought.
Well, both of these papers are really well done, I thought, and
they remind me of Yogi Berra who says it's not what you don't
know that hurts you. It's what you know that just ain't
true. And what is not brought up in these two papers probably is
a product of who we selected to have as our experts when we had
our memory experts in.
Dan and Jim are fascinated with the encoding part of the memory
story, but that's only half of the story. The other half of
the story is the recall of information, how you retrieve it, and
there's a whole rich literature on that.
And basically the point I want to make is that what we know about
that is that every time you retrieve the memory and it then becomes
relayed down as it were, re-remembered, the memory changes. So
it's the memory that you think you have that you are languishing
on and that is governing your current beliefs or mental thoughts
or what have you, really has been so buffed by you recalling it
through the past that if you actually went back to the original
event, you many times wouldn't recognize the two.
But your current memory of it and the actual event, and for those
of us who write scientific review papers, we have this problem all
the time. If we fail to go back and look up the source of the paper
that we actually are trying to write about in five years, we always
get it wrong because the memory, the recall system along with the
recoding of the information that's recalled, plays all of these
tricks on your mind.
So my point is that blunting of the memory, there's a natural
blunting of it that occurs just as we recall past events, and it's
just a part of the normal process of thinking about the past experience.
It's not that there's going to be this bullet of a drug
or a pharmaceutical that's going to come in and modify this
pristine, clear memory of a particular event.
And the only other thought, and I cannot speak for the rest of
the afternoon, is in the background paper that touched on memory
and aging and memory and loss of memory and so forth and the potential
pharmaceutical enhancements that could occur to help our lagging
memory system, the one point that's lost is that we all have,
all of us in our culture have lost a great skill set in our memory
tool box.
It used to be that I think we all in this room go back far enough
that we used to remember a verse at length and could repeat it and
use it constantly. I heard a little bit this morning, and I was
very impressed. And we've given that tool box up for the visual
culture we live in for all of the pneumonic aids we have all around
us.
And as a result, we're not going into the aging process particularly
well-equipped to continue the mental exercises, and so the deterioration
that is occurring through the normal aging process becomes exaggerated
because of looking at -- I'm not talking about the people in
this room.
CHAIRMAN KASS: No, no.
DR. GAZZANIGA: But our culture simply is not
applying this tool box of rehearsing information or remembering
it as they age, and certainly it is clinically known that the people
who are insulated the best against early Alzheimer's and all
the rest of it are the people who have kept mentally active and
are using their memory skill system with vigor.
And so therefore, the reason that I'm bringing that up is
that it's my bet that a behavioral resurgence of that will probably
even in the short- run be more productive in enhancing and protecting
us against deteriorating memory than the pharmaceuticals that are
imagined and that, in fact, now are being planned by one start-up
or another.
So those are my observations.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Mary Ann.
PROF. GLENDON: Can I just try to make a connection
between what Michael said and Gil's paper?
I think Michael reminds us quite rightly that this business of
memory is very tricky, but the thing that I want to -- one of the
many things I loved about Gil's paper is the way he connects
memory with the narrative of who we are, with the story of who we
are.
And I was trying to think about this problem of accuracy of memory
and connection with that narrative, and here the literary piece
that we need to put in our collection is Mario Vargas, "The
Storyteller," which is about a tribe that is losing its storytellers,
this Machiguenga tribe, calls itself "The People Who Walk,"
and they tell their stories. They have an official storyteller
who tells the story over and over again, but, Michael, every time
it's told it's told differently.
And the storyteller sometimes says, "Well, when the great
god exhaled us, perhaps he did this." And so there's this
perhaps that lets you know that the storyteller knows about it.
So what's happening is not only remembering, but sort of recreation
of the story as it's passed on, and I think the most -- even
though the subject here is memory, I think that Gil was right on
when he said that what connects this memory question to being human
is its connection to our stories about what our life means, the
search for meaning.
CHAIRMAN KASS: But I mean, let me make life
somewhat difficult. I mean, taking from Mike Gazzaniga's comment
the suggestion that, well -- I mean, it was quite wonderful. For
the problem of bad memories, time is the best remedy since it won't
bother you so much as you get older.
I wish it were true. There are still things that make me wince.
(Laughter.)
CHAIRMAN KASS: And the other part of it is memorized
poetry, never mind pharmacology, a sentiment which we could endorse
for multiple reasons. But it seems to me that if having a narrative
is a narrative of one's own life is somehow central to our own
humanity and self-understanding, and unless the retellings of it
are simply accidental in the way in which the messages and the telephone
game get changed inadvertently, it does suggest that one could be
an editor of one's own narrative and be increasingly the author
of our own life, the way we are at least to some extent.
If I don't say it, Bill May is going to say it. It's
not simply a matter of acquiescence and taking what comes, but it's
a matter of trying to shape and by acts of will form the life story
prospectively, and so the question is so why shouldn't one as
the partial author, not the exclusive author of one's life,
do a little editorial work to take out those things the memory of
which will besmirch, upset, trouble, cripple, I mean, fracture a
life?
Why should the narrative that we tell ourselves be somehow necessarily
faithful as much as memory could ever be faithful to what we actually
endured? I mean why not be good editors rather than let accident
write the story?
PROF. SANDEL: Let's see what Gil has to
say to that.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Huh?
PROF. SANDEL: Let's hear what Gil has to
say to that.
CHAIRMAN KASS: You should make him answer first.
PROF. MEILAENDER: That's an excellent idea.
I was just going to say why exactly did you want to make this case
for living in a world of fantasy. You know, this is Rousseau --
CHAIRMAN KASS: I'm just asking.
PROF. MEILAENDER: This is Rousseau walking across
Europe preferring to think of kind of the women he can conjure up
in his imagination to real women.
PROF. SANDEL: No. To tell a life story isn't
to be a beat reporter, Gil. That's how I understood Leon's
point.
PROF. MEILAENDER: No. You asked why should
one want to live truthfully.
DR. GAZZANIGA: Well, the point is that the truthfulness
isn't very good. I mean, but --
PROF. MEILAENDER: But, Leon, I acknowledge that
one could strive for it.
CHAIRMAN KASS: No, no, no, no.
PROF. MEILAENDER: Granting that none of us will
remember perfectly, you can still try or you can decide to kind
of fix up and get rid of. That was what --
PROF. SANDEL: But now you're suggesting,
Gil, that the truest life story, the truest narrative for a person
would be a transcript.
PROF. MEILAENDER: No, I wasn't. You see,
we're into the Q&A again. This is not the right way to
do it, and I'm just going to shut up in a minute.
The paper acknowledges that we do reconfigure inevitably and that,
indeed, one can't necessarily say what the meaning of an event
is until it's seen in the context of the whole life. That seems
to me to be true, or at any rate I certainly say it in the paper.
But simply taking charge of it in such a way as to kind of eliminate
those matters that one finds painful in some way doesn't seem
to me to be an attempt to live truthfully, and it doesn't take
seriously the fact that we don't know what the meaning of those
events can become in the course of a whole life as it's lived
out.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Did you want to follow up that?
PROF. SANDEL: Just one follow-up. If we agree
that a life story as a narrative is central to the meaning of a
life, and I think that point comes out powerfully in your paper,
Gil, and Mary Ann elaborated this idea, then we agree that a certain
element of creative fashioning is not only desirable. It's
essential if the life story is to be other than a stenographic transcript.
Now, if that's true, then the next step would be to ask:
well, that means being less moved by some facts about our lives
than others, seeing them as less central in the story that we weave,
and memory enters here because one of the ways in which we de-emphasize
certain features of our lives and emphasize others in weaving the
story may have to do with a certain faculty not just of will, not
just of saying, "Well, let's array all of the facts here
truthfully and then I'll pick out the significant ones,"
but it has to do with the way we take them in in the first place.
And memory even untouched by drugs is selective, and the principles
of selectivity may have something to do with bias and prejudice,
but may also have something to do with the distinctive way we take
in the world in the first place unless you think we just take it
in as brute sense data.
And so that suggests that even without the drugs memory is going
to be selective memory, selective in a way that reflects our distinctive
take on the world.
And so then the objection to altering memory, whether to blot
out traumatic memories or to increase our ability to remember certain
things on either direction might be seen as part of what we do anyhow
when we take in the world, and it might be odd to think that the
way we just happen to take in the world unaltered from either direction
is the past. We're back to the moral weight of the given.
Why should we think that that's necessarily going to lead
us to the truest life story?
PROF. MEILAENDER: I think it's peculiar
to use the word "selective" in connection with sort of
just what we take in. We haven't yet gotten to something that
deserves the word "selective" at that point. You know,
experience comes in. It's organized in various ways. I understand
that, but the notion of selecting in any strong sense doesn't
fit that.
PROF. SANDEL: No, but experience doesn't
just come in. That's the point.
PROF. MEILAENDER: I said it's organized
in certain ways, but the language is "selecting," I think,
is not very good language there.
PROF. SANDEL: Well, apprehending, and some people
are better at apprehending than others, more insightful. They're
--
PROF. MEILAENDER: Yes, but that's got nothing
to do with their conscious shaping of their sense of the narrative
of their life.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Bill May.
DR. MAY: Interesting to think about imagination
in relationship to memory. I mean if we didn't remember the
past at all, imagination would function in relationship to the future,
but it has very few controls on fantasy.
If I didn't know very much about the various inertias at work
in my life, I might fantasize all things, and so memory has -- and
your discussion of embodiment I think was very important there,
Gil. Memory in lots of ways serves to tether, tame, and restrain
imagination as it relates to the future.
On the other hand, imagination is very important in the functioning
of memory because, after all, it's passed and, therefore we
have to engage in an act of imaginative retrieval. So it's
very interesting.
The retrospective of imagination serves in various ways to constrain
the prospective side of imagination as we muddle our way through
the present moment.
Now, you use the word selection. But also there's re-collection,
is a very important feature, and when one thinks of the moral task
of old age -- I think about it more and more -- as it bears on this
whole question of coherence, there's a task of re-collecting
one's past if you're going to be concerned at all with the
question of coherence and think about life as a narrative.
There's a kind of re-collecting that goes on. That word,
I recall in root, "collect," "lectionary," and
so forth. It's a reading. It isn't simply a record. It's
a reading, and as long as we're alive, there is a rereading
that is going on in our relationship to the past.
But as you deal now with what you call the wince events, of which
they're very important because I discover I am more afflicted
by them when something else is going wrong. So they become symptoms
to me when I'm reliving certain kinds of things that, in fact,
there are other orders of disarray and incoherence in my present
life that makes me think about some of those wince events.
I mean, Paul McHugh will know a great deal more about this than
I do.
But Gil also introduces an interesting issue. How do you relate
to those very painful experiences or wince events, and what kind
of editing and selecting now, partial reading, where you remove
from the book, remove from the pages certain kinds of events to
deal with them, and that's one way of achieving a coherence,
but it's dealing not with the whole truth. We know that.
But Gil, on the basis of his religious background at some point
suggests that in this life we never have a vantage point that gives
us the whole truth, another way of saying there isn't the --
the final coherence is never ours, and I guess it is what has led
religious traditions to talk about the problems of reparation and
atonement; that there's a lot in our lives, even when we make
the best efforts at retrieval and collection and so forth that are
ragged, are frayed, jagged, that do not fit in, and so forth, and
we're engaged in unfinished business. And where does the completion
come from?
And oddly, humility requires us both to aspire for coherence,
but to be cautious in assuming that we ever achieve some final vantage
point on that, and, of course, that's why we need one another
to engage in that kind of correction in our readings, which are
so partial, so agenda- driven, so often, from day to day.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Does that that sort of large
and very moving anthropological account lead you to conclude that
we doctors of the soul, that is to say I'm speaking for McHugh,
not to come in there and do anesthesia of memory, not even for the
most painful of memories, if he could?
DR. FOSTER: Well, while he's thinking, I
mean -- I mean you all are -- I would myself think that that would
be a great loss. You know, I mean, Jacques Martain wrote about
this extensively with something that -- I forgot exactly who he
was quoting about the memories that come like lightning uninvited
in, you know, from the past.
If you can allow just a little homely example of this, when I
was about 16 years old or 17 years old, 16 years old, I think, I
got a very good job working in an electronic -- I was a sort of
a jack of all trades there and so forth, and one of the bizarre
things, there were two of us working there, and he asked me to come
work at his house on Saturday, which was not a day that I worked,
and I had a date. So instead of telling him that I had a date and
that I didn't think I could come, I made up a frank, bizarre
lie. I said that I had a brother who was coming home from the Service
and that I had to greet him.
(Laughter.)
DR. FOSTER: So my friend went out to work with
him, and my boss, who was a wonderful boss, said, "Well, Dan
couldn't come because his brother's coming from the Service,"
and whereupon my friend, Fred Kongabel, says, "Well, he doesn't
have any brother in the service.
(Laughter.)
DR. FOSTER: Right? Well, so the next day, and
to his credit, my boss, who was a wonderful person, confronted me
with this and in a reparative way. I thought he would fire me,
but he said, "I was very disappointed that you simply didn't
tell me the truth because I would have understood. I've had
dates before."
Well, to this day, that is one of the most painful memories that
I've ever -- I mean, it doesn't sound like much, you know.
It was just a little white lie because I didn't want to work
on Saturday, but it is constantly with me as a reminder. I sometimes
tell this story to the residents and so forth and so on. It reminds
me of the importance of truth.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Of?
DR. FOSTER: Of truth, of telling the truth.
And so whenever there's an opportunity where I feel like there's
something that I ought not to tell the truth because I don't
want to do something, and so forth and so on, that memory, painful
as it is, has served to remind me of the awful pain I felt because
I disappointed this man who had given me a job to do that.
So I think editing of painful things -- I have a lot of wince
-- I'm not going to tell you about some of them, you know, but
I'm pretty straight, you know. But still I have a lot of those.
I think it would be -- I think I agree that it would be a bad
thing. I think it's one thing to eliminate a memory of a plane
crash or like the bus that was blown up in Israel yesterday, and
you know, with people's faces drawn up. That might be, you
know, to delete something like that might be one thing, but if you
had to delete all of the other things from which you've actually
learned and probably gotten wisdom and probably finally become something
more close to what you wanted to be all your life, then I think
that would be bad because then it might be that I wouldn't have
that to remind me of things.
So in one sense one deals with those winces by, you know, the
forgiveness of friends or the person that you've had or, as
Bill says, maybe if there is some ultimate judgment in the universe
by that.
I mentioned this article this morning because I wanted to -- of
Leon Rosenberg's because I was really wanting to just talk about
the issue of whether one should just fight problems or deal with
them by drugs.
But even though I wanted it in the archives, I think every one
of us ought to read this because it's a confession in which
he dealt with and did not delete the memories of his suicide attempt,
of his inability to fight, and so forth. And that is now six years
beyond that, and he's, you know, restored, but in some sense
his need to not not remember, but to share that helped him to get
well, along with electroconvulsive therapy and things of that sort.
So I'm speaking too long, but I would be .- I think I would
be bereft in terms of the kind of life I want to live if I deleted
all of those things that were painful from which I tried to change
my life from it.
DR. MAY: I take it the purpose was to give me
time to think through an answer, Dan.
(Laughter.)
DR. FOSTER: That's what I -- I was trying to
help you, Bill.
DR. MAY: Thank you for your collegial service.
DR. FOSTER: Yes, that's right.
CHAIRMAN KASS: As long as you remember the question.
DR. MAY: Well, it was could you justify interventions,
I think is what you asked me, given this --
CHAIRMAN KASS: As a healer of souls, yes.
DR. MAY: Yeah, and traumatic memories can be
of an order to do something by way of offering provisional shelter
for them, it seems to me. One would have to be satisfied that we
know there are traumatic situations where people are so reliving
it that, in fact, much else in their past is wiped out, and so it
isn't as though you're simply wiping out the past. You
may be sheltering them from certain events in such a way to allow
them to reappreciate full ranges of the past that in their obsession
they are not able to relate to, but one would hope that it is a
limited and provisional shelter. One would hope in total coherence
to allow them even to face up to that event.
So it would seem to me a kind of limited provisional justification.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Let me bother you just one small
point further on this. It turns out, and it's mentioned, I
think, in Gil's paper and also in the staff paper that at least
with the memory blunting agents now under discussion, they would
have to be taken within a certain short period of time after the
event because this has to do with the consolidation.
DR. MAY: Right. The consent issues are there,
too.
CHAIRMAN KASS: No, not so much the consent question
as much as the question of do you think one would know on the occasion
whether this is the kind of an event deserving of such shelter for
the sake of the rest?
DR. MAY: I don't know. I certainly --
CHAIRMAN KASS: Or is there some sense that Gil's
remark about the unknowability of the meaning of any of these events
might lead you to say, well, we can't edit in advance?
DR. MAY: Yeah, I certainly think one would need
to be cautious because it would be very quickly, immediately justified
for managerial purposes that you would shoot them up in order to
manage them, and whether it was really serving them or whether it's
something that they could more directly face might tend to get institutionally
repressed.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Can we continue on these?
Let's see. Robby, on this?
PROF. GEORGE: Yeah, just on this because I thought
what Bill said was very good, but, Bill, I wonder if you would accept
a friendly amendment or maybe I've just misunderstood. I think
the way you summed it up at the end with what you were arguing for
was you said you were offering a limited provisional justification
for sometimes blocking.
DR. MAY: You can imagine certain -- yeah.
PROF. GEORGE: But it sounded to me like what
you were offering was something slightly different. Rather it was
a justification for a limited provisional blocking, the distinction
here begin, I take it, if I can kind of fill in the background to
make sense of that, it would be the idea that the -- I hate to put
it in such sterile, analytic terms, but if you'll forgive me
just for the moment that the truth of the proposition is always
a reason for knowing it, but it's certainly not always a conclusive
reason for adverting to it, especially in view of the fact that
very often propositions that we know and can actually avert to in
terms of memory are emotionally just awful.
So that in view of that and without in any way attacking the value
of truth itself, the value that makes the truth of a proposition
the reason for knowing it, we can have reasons for purposes of health,
psychological health, doing something to blunt the pathological
implications of our inability not to avert to that horrible memory
in this or that case.
Am I on your wave length or have I changed your --
DR. MAY: Yeah. No, I think that's right.
Timing is very important.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Michael and then Paul, Mary Ann.
Michael.
PROF. SANDEL: Bill, you were addressing half
of the question of intervention, the one you had been asked about.
Should the doctor of the soul over here intervene to block someone
forming a memory?
What about the opposite side of that question? Should he intervene
with the patient to dredge up for them a memory that they've
already blocked? Is there any reason why he should intervene or
not -- did the same reasons operate on both sides?
DR. MAY: Well, and Freud depended heavily on
the latter obviously.
PROF. SANDEL: But are the considerations different
or is this symmetrical?
DR. MAY: Well, and I felt part of the justification,
as I recall it, was that in failing to retrieve those memories,
one is often fated to engage in repetitive acting out because one
hasn't dealt with the buried memories. Is that right, Paul?
PROF. SANDEL: Maybe, but it may also be an act
of kindness or compassion or humanity for Paul to leave some of
those buried memories alone, wouldn't you say? Or is that always
a failure of respect because you're not bringing the person
into contact with the truth?
CHAIRMAN KASS: Well, let me make this slightly
more complicated, and forgive me for jumping the queue, but it's
a continuation of this thought.
One could make an argument in advance that there are certain things
that we would be better off not knowing. If you know what you're
doing, you don't really let people to go down and view the dismembered
bodies that have been strewn after an explosion, whatever their
voyeuristic curiosity might be.
And if you could make an argument that a certain kind of ignorance
might be salutary and preventable, therefore, what do you do if
someone accidentally stumbles upon a kind of knowledge which would
be terribly disorienting and produce all kinds of painful effects
through no fault of their own.
I mean, you know, Ham walks into the tent and sees the nakedness
of his father. Now, he shouldn't have been in the tent, but
we're better off if he hadn't seen it, and if we could somehow
blunt the memory of his father's disgrace when we would have
prevented him from witnessing it in the first place, why not?
PROF. SANDEL: Right. That's the --
CHAIRMAN KASS: That's the kind of line of
-- that's in keeping --
PROF. SANDEL: Exactly.
CHAIRMAN KASS: -- with your thought.
PROF. SANDEL: Yeah, and that seems to me the
bigger issue.
PROF. GEORGE: Well, you certainly don't
want to -- sorry to interrupt, Leon -- but on this I think that
the baseline from which we're operating here was provided by
Gil. We don't want to validate the idea that it's okay
to live a lie. We want to take as our baseline that people should
live, that a life well lived is a life lived in line with the truth.
But we're wondering if, consistent with that, it's sometimes
appropriate to blunt or block access to certain memory. So I think
we need to distinguish and see if we can put any analytic rigor
into distinguishing that blunting and blocking from falsification.
PROF. SANDEL: The truth ain't everything.
CHAIRMAN KASS: I'm sorry?
PROF. SANDEL: The truth ain't everything.
CHAIRMAN KASS: That I guess is what's under
discussion.
Paul and then --
DR. McHUGH: Yeah, I want to come back really
with where Michael began here and then come to what's being
discussed. The essence of memories are that we do affect them and
shape them and lose some of the details, but the interesting thing
is that we often keep the gist of the memories, a certain aspect
of gist is sustained.
So what is this biological function we have of remembering things
in which there is a fall-off of detail even though there is a conception
of gist?
And I work, after all, with people and their memories and the
like, and I can validate a lot of things that Bill is saying, including,
for example, that when they're in a particular state of mind
all of these nasty, wincing things come back up to them, and they'll
say things to me like, "Well, that's really me."
That was, you know, "I told a lie. I'm really a liar,"
or you know, "I went to a peep show and I'm really a sex
fiend," and things of that sort.
And that's usually coming out of the illness, depression,
the disease depression that picks out particular memories and gives
them tremendous salience, and I want to correct the depression,
and then when I do that, these memories fade back into wincing experiences
of the very sort that Dan is talking about.
But there's something else about having a memory capacity
that keeps a gist and loses some of the details because we work
with that in psychotherapy all the time. Again, I have people come
in to me with a whole variety of -- who will bring a life story
to me, you know. They bring the story, and they say, "Look.
I don't know if you can help me at all with this, but this is
the way I look at myself and look at my background."
And some of them, for example, bring in, you know, "I was
psychoanalyzed for a long time and spent all of my time wondering
about my Oedipal complexes and things of that sort."
And I sometimes say, "Well, why do you come to me after that?
You know you're going to get something else if you come to me."
But the issue is that we now know this about people, all people
seeking psychotherapy, and that is that they don't have the
same story. They don't have -- they're not all Oedipally
conflicted. They do not all, in fact, have sexual conflicts the
way Freud thought.
The thing that they have in common is that they're all in
the same state of demoralization. Somehow the way they are looking
at the world, the things that they have experienced, the things
that have happened to them have demoralized them and have lost for
them the capacity to go forward.
And my job is not to eliminate the memories that they have, but
to give them a different salience in their life. I mean, we've
just heard this wonderful story from Dan. Now, you told me a little
homely story. Suppose he had come to me and said, "You know,
Paul, there it is. You know, I'm still sick about it. Can
you help me?"
And he might come in and say, "Can you give me some Prozac?"
I would say, "No." I would say I want to look at that
story. The gist of it is correct, but I want to look at it as a
young fellow who learned something and who had the character to
be shamed by that and from that time on, his character, which is
the thing which has led him on into other things, is reflective
of his abilities and, therefore, give him hope, make him believe
in himself again.
You've got to believe Dan. You've got to believe that
you have wonderful things, and the essence of the memories that
allow things to fall away and yet keep a gist is that I can tell
a better story for the patient than the one that he brings in that
demoralizes him, and I give them a better way of looking at it.
Now, is it the truth? Well, since the story he brought in wasn't
the truth, then the story I'm giving him isn't what you
-- that was a wonderful -- isn't a secretarial truth, you know?
A day-by-day chronicle, it is.
He's got a view of it, and I have a view of it, and I want
to give him my view because I think it helps him believe in himself
and believe that he has mastery over the future rather than to be
demoralized by it.
And so I think the reason that we have this memory that is not
a videotape of the world is just for that reason. It allows us
to eventually get hope and develop it, sometimes by help from a
coach like me, but often by ourselves.
You know, Dan has come to terms with that. I'm not going
to tell you any of the wincing stories about mine. Oh, awful, but
we go on.
And so the third point I want to make is about these traumatic
events, and I'm very concerned about anything that's going
to try to eliminate the memory of the traumatic event. I'm
very happy to help somebody who has had a traumatic event and is
not sleeping, is anxious and agitated, is house-bound and things
of that sort. I see plenty of them, and I help them with those
symptoms even as I sympathize with them, with for example the loss
of a child or something of that sort.
Well, I'll help him or her with the symptoms, but I don't
want to take the memory away, not only because I think the memory
has value in them, in their life story and their chronicle, but
also I think that the memory reminds them of the dangers that they're
in and what they need to -- you know, these other symptoms give
us a chance to avoid things.
And then to finally come back to this idea-- Do you want to dredge
up memories because you think they may be the pathological ones?
Well, that is what has provoked the so-called memory wars of the
'90s in which the theory was if you have these moods, you must
have something buried, and it's going to be sexual, and it's
going to be this.
And, again, we could install that, but that turned out not to
be so much a memory as a belief. These people were not -- they
called it, "Well, now I remember," but actually it was
a belief, and you can even see different things light up in PET
scans on them as they try to pull it up.
And our job is not to instill those kinds of false beliefs in
them, particularly a false belief that encourages the view that
you're a victim. I don't want to make anybody feel they're
a victim. We've all been victimized, and little guys have been
victimized more than tall guys.
But we don't want --
(Laughter.)
DR. McHUGH: But we don't want to live our
lives and go forward just remembering or thinking about ourselves
as victims and these recovered beliefs and all, finally the memory
wars are over, but they were an effort to produce beliefs that were
not good stories. They were stories that ultimately invalidated
people.
So I'm coming back, coming around to the other thing. Would
I want to eliminate a painful memory from somebody? Not really.
I would like to relieve the pain if I could, make it less, but I
want people to have the gist of their memories, and then they may
need help in shaping them in ways that continue to make them feel
they still have mastery over their future.
PROF. GLENDON: What I have to say actually is
related to what Paul said. I agree with your cautiousness about
not wanting to take away a traumatic memory, but I wonder if you
would accept a distinction between the kind of trauma that a rescue
worker may experience in that nightclub fire in Rhode Island or
in the World Trade Center?
And it seems to me between that kind of trauma and the trauma
that is directly tied up with one's life story, the loss of
a loved one under violent circumstances, it seems to me I'm
not altogether persuaded that it wouldn't be appropriate to
help the rescue worker take the pill, you know, even preventively
because they know from photographs and other things in their training.
They know what they're going to see, but do they need to see
90 -- well, you know, we know what happened there.
DR. McHUGH: Well, it's interesting that
there are data on that in relationship to a major, major disaster,
the disaster known as the Piper Alpha disaster, the fire in a North
Sea oil rig in which it turned out that there were rescue workers
later that went in and brought the bodies out.
And the thing was that the rescue workers had been examined prior
for other reasons. They had been given psychological assessments,
and there were then two groups of rescue workers, ones that were
just given no assistance, just thrown into the bodies, the morgues,
and the other one in which the psychiatrists in Aberdeen were with
them throughout, reminding them of the good that was coming of bringing
these bodies out for the families, giving them a continuing story
about this horror.
And the interesting thing was this second group of people -- we
talk about grief counselors going in trying to get people out.
They're no use, but if you can give a story to people during
their rescue work and recovery work, they manage it.
They put it into there and they said, "No, this is a stinking,
dirty, violent work, but it means something to somebody else,"
and that helps them, and you can do that without a pill if you can
make it clear and help people in that process.
The Piper Alpha disaster story was spelled out by the psychiatrist,
and it's an absolutely fascinating subject in relationship to
traumatic memories.
DR. GAZZANIGA: So we can't be simplistic
about this, and we've done the typical thing that we all do,
which is take this and plant it, I mean this traumatic memory and
this drug, and we're going to block the traumatic memory, et
cetera, et cetera, et cetera, and that's just not how it works.
That's what I'm trying to tell you. It's a continual
updating of an event that gets changed naturally through time.
And the second point is that 90 percent of the people who have
a traumatic experience have no problems later on in life of a psychiatric
nature, and that what the event does in many instances is it simply
triggers a predisposition for a mental condition that will find
them at one time or another probably winding up in Paul's office.
So you can't just take this little simple view of the brain
that there's this memory. You can encode it. You can block
it. You can do this; you can do that. There's a lot of overhead,
of mental overhead in the psychological structure of the person.
Maybe the people who aren't traumatized by this have already
told themselves the story. There's obviously a reason to go
in and get these people out, and so they naturally self-administered
this sort of thing.
But finally, just to -- yeah?
CHAIRMAN KASS: On the first point, you're
now suggesting not only does memory get modified through the retelling
and revisiting, but are you also saying that the suggestions of
the sort that we hear and that have, in fact, been reported in a
way in the literature, that even that is rather simplistic; that
there is no such thing as in the process of memory consolidation
altering the affective character of the way that gets remembered?
Just as a factual question.
DR. GAZZANIGA: There are people who react to
-- you take 100 people and show the same traumatic event, how they
react and what they take away from it varies all over the place.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Agreed, but is there no way to
intervene on the occasion?
DR. GAZZANIGA: Oh, there are these manipulations
as reported, yes. Yeah, yeah, yeah.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Okay.
DR. GAZZANIGA: No, I'm not saying that.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Okay.
DR. GAZZANIGA: Let me go to Paul's point
on this because this is one I happen to know about. The danger
of the implanted memory that we all -- the psychiatrists certainly
worry about this as they talk to people, and culturally there's
a social psychologist new school, Henri Zukier, who was doing a
study on the Holocaust syndrome, and he noticed that he could understand
the Holocaust syndrome being a reality, but his suspicions began
to grow when the children of the Holocaust event had the Holocaust
syndrome.
And then finally the coup de gras was the grandchildren of the
Holocaust victim. He said, "How does this work? You know,
we're now two steps away from it."
So he said he thought he'd go investigate the Holocaust syndrome,
and he said, "I'll go to Europe where obviously it would
be intense," and he went to the psychiatric communities of
Europe on the Holocaust, and they said, "What are you talking
about? There's no Holocaust syndrome."
And he went and did all of the proper scholarship of this and
discovered this was an invention of New York psychiatrists that
this existed, and they built the syndrome and people came, and in
so much of what we're talking about here, we are building syndromes
and people come and they are assigned into these categories, and
then we as a culture have to live with it, and that's a big
problem.
DR. McHUGH: And we put them in DSM-IV, by the
way, operational criteria.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Gil and then Bill and then Michael.
PROF. MEILAENDER: Comments on a couple of things.
On the rescue worker issue, if you think that Mary Ann was building
up sympathy for, and I can understand it, but if you think about
it, we're going to have these people take the pill preemptively,
so to speak. That blunts the memory.
Part of what's going on is you wouldn't think that someone
could go through the whole of his life doing this. I mean, imagine
just a lifetime spent as a rescue worker dealing with the horror
of dismembered bodies. That suggests to me that maybe nobody should
do this for his life, for a whole life, you see, because otherwise
the notion is that it would be fine to sort of create a group of
people who, as it were, could do this for their whole life and not
be bothered by it.
That seems to me much more troubling than the notion of, you know,
letting some people have these memories and then trying to help
them deal with it.
So, I mean, if you think about it from that perspective, it seems
to me it raises questions about doing it, and I thought that Paul's
example was nice in that it suggested a way of dealing redemptively,
to use the category that I use in the paper. So that's one
thing on the rescue worker.
And then the other thing, just the question. I mean, I assume
that there are some things that it would be better not to know probably.
The question is how you are to live if you do know them. That's
the question, how you are to live truthfully as a human being.
And I don't think that my claim in the paper was that one
lives truthfully in the face of that just by passively accepting
it, saying, "Well, there it is. I know it sort of," or
something like that, but rather that one lives humanly with it perhaps
simply by sort of stoic fashion, enduring and bearing up under it;
perhaps by gradually finding a way of telling the story that transforms
it in such a way that it gives it redemptive significance and either
or both of those may need the help of other people in order to accomplish.
It's not just something one does.
So those are sort of humanly fitting ways, it seems to me, that
one lives truthfully with such events. They're not the passive
acceptance of it, but neither are they denying that this is now
part of who I am and I need to find a way to deal with that as part
of who I am.
CHAIRMAN KASS: And for those people who are
not blessed either with faith and redemption or the strengths to
be stoics and for whom this is just pain, disorienting pain, and
who, by the way, may not have community that can help them deal
with this, suffer little children.
PROF. MEILAENDER: You can always make a problem
go away and free ourselves of the responsibility to be the community
that helps them by hypothesizing such circumstances, but, I mean,
it's just there might be such circumstances, but I wouldn't
create a policy. I wouldn't create my general attitude toward
the question on the basis of that because, like I say, that let
us off too easily. It lets us off the hook of trying to find ways
to serve their need.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Bill Hurlbut -- on this?
PROF. DRESSER: Just indirectly. It seems there
are two issues. One is do we want to concede that there are cases
where this would be the best thing for the individual, and I remember
-- maybe I'm misremembering -- but I think Dr. McGaugh talked
about the post traumatic stress disorder, the Vietnam vets who were
really paralyzed. I mean they could have no life, and he seemed
very moved by those cases.
So I wonder if we want to say that that would be a case where
we might think it could be justified.
But then the second question as Gil puts it is it seems if you
want to frame the intervention as, well, the only way this is going
to work is we have to give this to everyone after the Vietnam War
or while they're having these horrible experiences, and we know
there's, you know, three percent of them who will be able to
function after this because then all of these other people won't
be able to process the memories in a way that we think is meaningful.
DR. FOSTER: But remember that you have to use
these drugs either before the event or immediately afterwards.
I mean, you can't come back ten years later. Then if you have
a deletion, you delete all memories instead of the very painful
one.
So you'd have to say, as I think he said, that maybe we're
going to give beta blockers to people who are going into the dessert
here ahead of time. So the big problem that they talk about, the
deletion requires anticipation or immediate intervention within,
you know, a very short time.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Bill Hurlbut and then Michael.
DR. HURLBUT: When I read this, when I read Gil's
paper, it struck me there was a lot of implicit reference back to
Leon's paper from last week or last time we met. The notion
that we need to keep life coherent so that we can participate in
the intelligible world.
I want to make a comment, but I want to ask you a question on
the way. Is there such a thing, Paul, as a memory that in a --
let's talk about healthy adults for the moment -- that cannot
become like the grain of sand in the oyster that can't become
something richer and deeper? Are there truly destructive memories?
DR. McHUGH: Well, you know, I think you can
have a memory that you make destructive, and you certainly can have
an event that you remember that alters your life script. I mean,
let me just tell you, for example, we now know that the sexual abuse
of children by an adult which has been summed up often in the traumatic
area, the problem that it produces later in life, and it does produce
plenty of problems later in life, has less to do with the trauma
or even the memory of that event and everything to do with the change
in attitude that that child takes about sex itself.
Okay? We were just talking about that before. That child, because
of the adults -- by the way, it has to be an adult. It's not
some playing doctor and peer interactions -- an adult that comes
in and breaks into the relatively latency period of childhood, that
memory teaches that child some things about sexual life that are
destructive. There should be more partners, and they have more
partners; that they have very early consensual sexual experiences.
They develop sexual diseases. They have early pregnancies and the
like, and the bad outcome that we know about has more to do with
how the script changes because of this event, not the memory.
But if you looked at it, the memory has produced the script, and
so there are four. It's a bad thing, but what the psychiatrists
do now for these children, in my opinion, is not that he goes in
and tries to eliminate this awful memory, and it's awful that
the person has been so intruded into, but they immediately begin
to start laying out, well, let's look at the way we'd really
like you to think as a person about this very important part of
life that you're going to have to grow to, and the thing that
has happened to you has made you vulnerable to a pernicious and
potentially very pathological life course.
DR. HURLBUT: So that's the key then, isn't
it? The script that you, the story that you contain your events
of your life in is the story that you want to sustain in its intelligibility
and work toward a deeper comprehensive understanding of the world
as you go, as you go forward. That's what the human soul is.
And it seems to me that beneath so many of the questions we've
been discussing for the last six months is hidden and we haven't
spoken of it very much, but is the question of what is the role
of disturbing suffering in this formation of the life story. Over
and over it just seems like biotechnology is offering us the opportunity
to avoid that which is uncomfortable or eroding to our self-esteem
or bad memories or, you know, no decline in aging.
It's interesting. Earlier when we were talking about the
compression of morbidity and so forth, it stirred a memory, a good
memory. I read that Vincent Van Gogh wrote a letter to his brother
Theo saying that he didn't want to die suddenly and comfortably.
He wanted to die of a very wrenching disorder like tuberculosis
or whatever it was in his vision, but he said that that way it would
be like a booster rocket that would send him off in the trajectory
of eternity.
And I'm considering how the guy died. It's a very poignant
thing to have said, but what strikes me is that at least -- and
what's so powerful about his art is that you feel the suffering
-- at least I do -- feel the suffering in his art, in his life,
and I for one don't want to tell myself a story that's a
fiction. I would like to go to the bottom of the story, and I think
at least somebody in our society has to face the very worst there
is in human existence or we're all in trouble.
And probably all of us, to the degree we're capable of it
at least, and whether there's some place for childhood and not
having memories or something, but the overall thrust of the thing,
it seems to me, ought to be to participate in this intelligible
language of being, which includes suffering, and to more and more
understand our lives at the depths of the reality of the world.
I was wondering what we're doing on this side of the river,
and I was think that maybe, when we were running last night -- I
ran by the cemetery -- and I was thinking, "Is Leon such a
thoughtful genius that he put us out here next to this memorial
of human's noble sacrifice?"
Because finally in the end, we know that's what makes a meaningful
life, is whether you're called to it or not, that finally you
go that deep, that you know what the world is, that the world involves
suffering, and you don't do what they did in Brave New World,
which was, you know, basically "Christianity without tears."
As they said, "Nothing costs enough here."
And the one line that kept coming back to me from Leon's paper
was, that finally human flourishing rooted is aspiration born of
deficiency. Well, I think we need to for the fullest lives go to
the fullest depths of that deficiency.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Michael, and then I think unless
there's someone in the queue -- Michael, Alfonso, and I think
we'll call a break.
So thank you.
PROF. SANDEL: Listening to the discussion, I
think I've heard two different kinds of arguments against this
memory blocking pill, and I'd like to distinguish them. One
of them seems to base the objection on the idea of the deliberate
forgetting, that to take the pill to forget a trauma is to artificially
willfully forget.
And moreover, since we have to take it at the moment, we're
not in a good position to decide then whether it's the sort
of thing we should forget or suppress. That's one objection.
A second objection goes deeper and depends on the claim that it's
always good to know and face the truth, and this stronger claim
seems to me questionable, and it's questionable for reasons,
Leon, brought out by your example, a biblical one, but there are
lots of truths that we every day try to spare our children or loved
ones.
Sometimes when the newspaper has a particularly gruesome photo,
I take it and hide it not only from my kids, but even from my wife
just to spare them. Now, I'm depriving them of a certain truth.
Now, I don't know if I would conclude from that that if they
did happen to see it before I removed it I would give them a pill
to forget it, but is there a difference in principle between the
first impulse and the second?
But here's a way of testing whether these two reasons go together
or whether it's possible to distinguish, whether it's really
the deliberate, willful character of it that's objectionable.
I mean, this is may suggestion or hypothesis. Here might be a way
of testing that.
Suppose we remove the deliberate, willful, artificial aspect of
the dulling of memory, and consider a case where not a pill, but
nature systematically dulls a traumatic memory.
Now, I don't know how scientifically well founded this is,
but there is a folk- lore that the memory, the full memory of the
pain of childbirth is dulled, and that if it weren't, there
would be fewer children born, and we were told this -- I don't
know .- when we went to child- bearing classes, you know, where
they teach you, and this is purely anecdotal, but my wife completely
believes and supports this anecdotally at least, that if women really
did remember from one child to the next they would have as many
kids.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Scopolamine, I think is what
it is, right?
PROF. SANDEL: Now, here would be a case, if
this piece of common folk- lore is true, where it would be nature,
not a pill- dulling traumatic memory. Would we regard that as regrettable?
CHAIRMAN KASS: Gil, do you want this?
PROF. MEILAENDER: I don't know quite where
your piece of folklore comes from, but I know that there's a
story that the rabbis supposedly told asking why a woman had to
bring a certain kind of ritual offering after giving birth. It
probably had to do something with ritual purity and so forth.
But the answer the rabbis gave was that it was to atone for a
vow she made but never really intended to keep. That is to say
when she was in the pangs of labor, she vowed never again, but when
the child was born, that is to say when she saw what the event meant
in the light of the whole or at least a fuller story, saw the fruition
that gave real redemptive meaning to the labor, she realized that
she never intended to keep that.
Now, that's not a bad example of what we've been talking
about.
PROF. SANDEL: Well, Gill, neither you nor I
may be in the best position to testify on this question. So I would
still put the hypothetical. If nature dulls traumatic memory in
the case of the pain of childbirth, is that something to be regretted?
(Laughter.)
DR. FOSTER: Let me just say scientifically --
CHAIRMAN KASS: We now have an authority. Mary
Ann.
DR. FOSTER: Just to answer your question.
PROF. GLENDON: Well, I have to disagree with
you, Michael, in characterizing it as a traumatic memory. I think,
first of all, pain, and the medical people know better than I do,
but I don't think we remember pain, physical pain of any sort
quite the way -- I mean, that's a funny thing, that memory of
physical pain, and I think it is quickly forgotten.
I mean, you know that there was a painful experience, but it's
not like the memory that made us all wince when somebody mentioned,
the mere mention of the wince factor all around the table was a
shudder because we all have those vivid memories of the wince, which
are more vivid, I think, than the pain of childbirth.
DR. FOSTER: Well, what I was trying to say is
that most scientists think that those are the release of endorphins
and enkephalins, you know. We don't have an opiate receptor
in the pain so that you can take heroin. There are many people
who come through great trauma that have burns and so forth who have
no, as Mary Ann says, who have no or little memory of the pain,
and so we do have these endogenous opiate-like molecules that are
released.
And I think that what she said is probably exactly right.
PROF. SANDEL: But do they dull the pain or did
they dull the memory of the pain, Dan?
DR. FOSTER: I can't answer that. I presume
that because of their biologicals, they probably dulled the pain
more than the memory of it, I would guess.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Alfonso, take the last, and we'll
call a halt.
DR. GÓMEZ-LOBO: Yeah, actually this hooks
up with some of the stuff that Michael was saying. Let me start
with a question about nature, if nature dulls it.
You know, the more I hear about enhancement around this table
and the lot I heard last week in the conference on the Future of
Life out in Monterey, the more I tend to think that we, I mean,
humanity may not be on the right path.
One thing --
(Laughter.)
DR. GÓMEZ-LOBO: -- one thing that strikes
me as deeply wrong is precisely the malleability about which Professor
Pinker was talking. It's this sense that everything can be
changed, like we can walk in there and, you know, change virtually
everything.
Now, it seems to me that or I would like to retain the idea that
nature is a very complex, marvelous system of causality with a few
glitches. Surely there is illness, and there are very good reasons
to work against it.
But there are other domains where I just don't see the point
of going into it. Now, some of you are going to accuse me of unilateral
disarmament, and that may be a correct description. I tend to unilateral
disarmament myself because I think I should measure thing against
my own personality, my own authenticity and not necessarily against
my competitors.
Now, that said, I would delighted with Gil's paper not only
because of its depth, but because it provided, it seems to me,
all sorts of arguments to say let's leave this domain untouched.
One of the great reasons, for instance, that I saw -- and this
is really for me going to be a memorable phrase -- "not to
remember the face of evil is to miss the evil of which we ourselves
are capable."
Now, I'm not a psychiatrist, and I can understand that there
can be traumatic memories, but, on the other hand, I think it's
very important to remember certain things.
I myself lived on the road to Dachau for almost two years in Munich,
and I used to take visitors to the camp. I suddenly discovered
that I was no longer taking anyone there after, you know, about
half a year, but it's one of those things that in a way I'm
grateful I haven't forgotten.
So I would say efforts -- I know this is very difficult, but it
seems to me that efforts should be directed to trying to draw lines
between therapy and the domain beyond therapy and really think whether
we're doing something reasonable in all of these efforts, which
by the way don't see to be very fruitful from what I've
gathered.
I mean, we haven't made much progress in suppression of memory,
if I understood correctly the two papers. We don't seem to
be very close to getting to designer babies, and so I know this
is very emotional on my part, but I think that there's something
very deep to think about here, and it is what are the human goals
that ultimately are going to be worthwhile. Are we really distancing
ourselves from nature and under the illusion that we can do better
than nature?
I have serious doubts about that.
CHAIRMAN KASS: Thank you very much.
One sentence to two sentences. This will be the last formally
scheduled, I think, discussion of this topic. For some of you that's
extremely good news.
I think if I might be indulged an editorial word, it's true
that some of these vaunted technologies are at the moment at least
less than they're cracked up to be when announced in the newspapers,
and that's a useful thing to have learned.
But in all aspects of this beyond therapy project, I think we've
seen how these new powers, in fact, do touch upon all kinds of things
that are really quite important, and the number of things that we've
talked about, whether it's the life cycle or the relation to
our bodies or the question of memory and the story of the life,
these really are the essential features, it seems to me, of at
least part of a richer bioethic.
And I would like to think that a serious discussion of those things,
whether it justifies the expenditure of the taxpayers' money,
and certainly it's not going to lead to any immediate public
policy questions, but it does seem to me that there's an opportunity
to do some kind of education of what might be at stake here, where
we are, in fact, from knowing what to do, and even a certain cautionary
note about whether we know what we were doing if we were doing it.
That I think is to be the spirit of what we will try to write
up and circulate amongst you for discussion, if all goes well, before
our next meeting.
Tomorrow morning, beginning at 8:30 we have two presentations
on the self-regulation of the assisted reproduction profession/industry.
We have presenters, and this is a return now to the project on regulation
and public policy, and it should be really quite, quite interesting.
Let me urge people to try to be on time at 8:30 because we will
have a guest to present.
You are free for the evening. You will not have to stand in line
to get your dinner bill, you know, paid for for an hour, and I
wish you good evening, and we'll see each other in the morning.
(Whereupon, at 5:31 p.m., the meeting in the
above-entitled matter was adjourned, to reconvene at 8:30 a.m.,
Friday, March 7, 2003.)
|